Vision and Cognition Lab
@vision-cognition.bsky.social
3 followers 1 following 9 posts
Vision and Cognition Lab, University of Tübingen
Posts Media Videos Starter Packs
vision-cognition.bsky.social
In short: color and space aren’t processed in isolation. They’re intertwined in large-scale, region-specific maps that generalize across humans. This opens questions about adaptation of color processing possibly to optimize object- and scene perception in natural vision.
vision-cognition.bsky.social
Why does this matter? It suggests functional or evolutionary pressures have organized color coding in a spatially structured, conserved way—hinting at deep links between how we perceive where things are and what color they are.
vision-cognition.bsky.social
Those region-specific biases were shared across brains. Meaning the way your V1 vs. V4 links space and color looks remarkably like mine, despite individual variability.
vision-cognition.bsky.social
This worked across multiple visual areas (V1–V3, hV4, LO1-2). Importantly, each area showed its own idiosyncratic color biases across retinotopic space. So different regions map color onto space in their own unique—but consistent—ways.
vision-cognition.bsky.social
Then we tested whether we could predict what color someone was seeing based only on brain activity patterns from other people’s brains. A classifier trained on others’ color responses could decode colors in a new subject.
vision-cognition.bsky.social
We used fMRI and a clever trick: align brains based only on responses to achromatic spatial patterns used for retinotopic mapping across individuals, without using any color information.
vision-cognition.bsky.social
The questions we asked: does the same color trigger comparable neural activity across different people? And do brain regions encode colors in distinct, systematic ways? We set out to test this directly.
vision-cognition.bsky.social
The human brain’s color responses are similar across individuals, forming large-scale, region-specific maps.